|
Allan W. Snyder* and D. John Mitchell
Centre for the Mind, Institute of Advanced Studies
The Australian
National University, Canberra, Australia 0200
Unlike the ability to acquire our native language, we struggle
to learn multiplication and division. It may then come as a surprise
that the mental machinery for performing lightning fast integer
arithmetic calculations could be within us all even though it
can not be readily accessed, nor do we have any idea of its primary
function. We are led to this provocative hypothesis by analysing
the extraordinary skills of autistic savants. In our view such
individuals have privileged access to lower levels of information
not normally available through introspection.
keywords: savants, autistic savants, unconscious arithmetic,
mental arithmetic, cognition, mind, consciousness, brain sciences,
consciousness
* author for correspondence A.W. Snyder, FRS
email: a.snyder@anu.edu.au
1. Introduction
We are largely unaware of the ways in which our brains process
information. For example, we are not conscious that shape is computed
from object shading or that perspective is derived (among various
ways) by the gradient of texture (Hemholtz 1910, Snyder and Barlow
1988). And why should we be? It is the "object" itself
that is of ultimate interest rather than the manner in which we
derive its label. Indeed, this specific example explains why it
is so difficult to draw naturally occurring scenes, as has been
elaborated elsewhere (Snyder and Barlow 1988, Snyder and Thomas
1997). Analogously, the foundations of our most fundamental beliefs,
our mindsets, are not normally available for introspection. We
are highly concept driven (Bartlett 1932; Snyder 1998). Presumably,
this confers advantage by allowing us to operate automatically.
Essentially, a world of unconscious information is sifted through,
by mechanisms of which we are unaware, to arrive at our final
judgements. We might therefore be in for some genuine surprises
if we had access to the mental processes used to construct our
mindsets.
To gain insight into this fundamental problem we turn to a rare
group of individuals, savants with early infantile autism, because
they appear significantly less concept driven than normal individuals.
Furthermore, this group has been a subject of scrutiny, so we
can borrow from comprehensive empirical studies and much theoretical
discussion (Kanner 1943; Asperger 1944; Frith 1989; Treffert 1989;
Baron-Cohen 1994) in order to build a framework from which predictions
can be made about unconscious mental processes. This leads to
our hypothesis that the mental machinery for performing lightning
fast integer arithmetic (lengthy multiplication, division, factorization
and prime identification) is within us all, although it can not
normally be accessed, nor do we know what primary function it
serves.
2. Savants - minds with privileged access to lower levels of
information?
Building on the pioneering work of Kanner 1943, Asperger 1944,
Frith 1989, O'Connor 1989 and Hermelin and O'Connor 1990, we surmise
that children with early infantile autism give insight into a
mind with limited mindsets, a mind that is not concept driven
(Snyder and Thomas 1997; Snyder 1998). In our view such a mind
can tap into lower level details not readily available to introspection
by normal individuals. This is consistent with the constellation
of traits associated with early infantile autism, especially those
of savants (Hill 1978; Treffert 1989; O'Connor 1989; Howe 1989;
Nettlebeck and Young in press) who seem to be aware of information
in some raw or interim state prior to it being formed into the
"ultimate picture". For example, it explains (Snyder
and Thomas 1997) how it is that Nadia (Selfe 1977), a mentally
retarded three and a half year old, can draw natural scenes like
that of Fig. 1 from memory, with astonishing life-like perspective
and to do so "spontaneously" without any training or
without even passing through the usual scribble stage.
Now, it is a surprising fact that normal individuals cannot draw
naturalistic scenes unless they are taught the tricks and schema
to do so (Gombrich 1960). The reason why this is so unexpected
is that our brains obviously possess all of the necessary visual
information required to draw, but we are apparently unable to
access it for the purpose of drawing. For example, our brain performs
the calculations necessary to label three-dimensional objects.
Yet the difficulties of drawing even a simple sphere are legion.
We are not consciously aware of how our brains derive shape from
shading, perspective from gradients of texture, size invariance
with distance and so on.
Fig 1 Autistic child's drawing at about three and a half
years (Selfe 1977).
Clearly, it is the object label or symbolic identification that
is of ultimate importance and not the actual attributes processed
by the brain to formulate the label (Snyder and Barlow 1988).
Indeed, normal preschool children draw, not so much what they
see, but rather from what can be called their mental schema. And,
these schema, like those of Fig. 2, tend to be invariant across
cultures. The horse is conventionally drawn side-on, head to the
left, and in bold outline form as is typical of late preschool
art. Somehow, the autistic savant Nadia can directly tap the way
in which our brain derives perspective, whereas normal individuals
can not. Yet these autistic savant artists often struggle to recognize
familiar faces (Selfe 1977).
So we believe that artistic savants have direct access to "lower"
levels of neural information prior to it being integrated into
the holistic picture - the ultimate label. All of us possess this
same "lower" level information, but we can not normally
access it. Ramachandran (1998 p. 287) in his remarkable new book
enriches this possibility by suggeting neurobiological mechanisms.
In our opinion, all savant skills can be explained as analogous
to those of drawing (Snyder, 1998). Put simply, savants have privileged
access to lower levels of "raw" information. Take the
ability of perfect pitch as an example. Our mechanism for hearing
consists of discrete frequency analysers which allow for the possibility
of perfect pitch. But, surprisingly only one in 10,000 persons
possess absolute pitch (Profita and Bidder 1988) and it is debatable
whether or not absolute pitch can be taught (Takeuchi and Hulse
1993). So, in analogy to our discussion above on vision, it is
the holistic information content that is important for hearing
and not the component attributes from which this is derived (Miller
1989; Heaton, Hermelin and Pring 1998). Yet, all musical savants
possess absolute pitch (Miller 1989). They apparently have access
to lower levels of auditory information while we do not.
These and other savant skills (Treffert 1989; Rimland and Fein
1988), including the extraordinary ability to recall seemingly
meaningless detail as opposed to recall of concepts, unusual sensory
discrimination of smell and touch, and even time keeping abilities,
reinforce our view that savants are able to tap something that
is in us all, but which is not normally accessible. And this is
consistent with numerous observations as captured by O'Connor's
(1989, page 4) statements that their "gift springs so to
speak from the ground, unbidden, apparently untrained and at the
age of somewhere between 5 and 8 years of age. There is often
no family history of the talent" and it "is apparently
not improved by practice." Also, the talents are "chiefly
in the direction of imitation and there is little capacity for
originality or for creativity" (Treffert 1989, page 9).
Fig 2 Representative drawings of normal children, each
at age four years and two months. (Emma and Teneal, Parents on
Campus Preschool, Australian National University).
To our knowledge no young savant (when the skill first
emerges) has ever given any insight into the methods used, nor
can they learn or be taught. With maturity the occasionally offered
insights are suspect, possibly being contaminated by expectations
or the acquisition of concepts concerning the particular skill.
Furthermore, savant skills often recede or are lost altogether
with the onset of maturity (Selfe 1977; Treffert 1989; Barnes
and Earnshaw 1995).
All of this suggests that the unusual skills of savants can be
used as a diagnostic tool to probe information from lower level
mechanisms which is not available to introspection of the normal
mind. But, the savant has not revealed unknown or unexpected mechanisms
in the case of drawing or perfect pitch. The physics of natural
scenes already tells us how perspective must be computed by the
brain and discrete frequency analysers are already known to be
the primary auditory receptors. Nor, in this vein, should the
savants' astonishing feats of recall for detail reveal anything
new about mental processing, since much evidence supports the
view that we all store an enormous amount of information, with
only a minute subset available for recall (Treffert 1989, Penfield
and Roberts 1966). Indeed, our recall like our drawing skills
appear to be concept oriented (Bartlet 1932).
So the extraordinary drawing skills of savants, their astonishing
recall of detail and their ability of perfect pitch do not reveal
unexpected mental processing. We all have the same raw information
but just can not directly access it, at least on call. But what
does the existence of savant lightning calculators tell us about
mental processing in the normal mind?
3. Savant lightning calculators.
Because normal children struggle to learn multiplication and
division, it is surprising that some savants perform integer arithmetic
calculations mentally at "lightning" speeds (Treffert
1989, Myers 1903, Hill 1978, Smith 1983, Sacks 1985, Hermelin
and O'Connor 1990, Welling 1994, Sullivan 1992). They do so unconsciously,
without any apparent training, typically without being able to
report on their methods, and often at an age when the normal child
is struggling with elementary arithmetic concepts (O'Connor 1989).
Examples include multiplying, factoring, dividing and identifying
primes of six (and more) digits in a matter of seconds as well
as specifying the number of objects (more than one hundred) at
a glance. For example, one savant (Hill 1978) could give the cube
root of a six figure number in 5 seconds and he could double 8,388,628
twenty four times to obtain 140,737,488,355,328 in several seconds.
Joseph (Sullivan 1992), the inspiration for the film "Rain
Man" about an autistic savant, could spontaneously answer
"what number times what number gives 1234567890" by
stating "9 times 137,174,210". Sacks (1985) observed
autistic twins who could exchange prime numbers in excess of eight
figures, possibly even 20 figures, and who could "see"
the number of many objects at a glance. When a box of 111 matches
fell to the floor the twins cried out 111 and 37, 37, 37. Similar
skills were reported as early as 1801 about a child named Dase,
who was also "singularly devoid of mathematical insight"
and of low general intelligence (Treffert 1989, Myers 1903).
4. Is integer arithmetic fundamental to mental processing?
If, as we believe, all savant skills have a common origin, then
the skill for integer arithmetic, (like that for drawing, perfect
pitch, and recall for meaningless detail), arises from an ability
to access some mental process which is common to us all, but which
is not readily accessible by normal individuals.
From this reasoning, we believe that everyone has the underlying
facility for performing lightning fast integer arithmetic. This
facility can not normally be tapped for the purpose of arithmetic
nor do we have any idea of its primary function. Rather, we must
learn arithmetic the way we learn to draw naturalistic scenes,
by implementing tricks and algorithms (Gombrich 1960; Snyder and
Barlow 1988; Snyder and Thomas 1997). Learning arithmetic is hard
work for normal individuals (Dehaene 1997), whereas it seems effortless
for mathematical savants. Why this should be is deeply mysterious.
As with drawing, tricks and algorithms can be learned for doing
rapid arithmetic, but some savant lightning calculators vastly
out-perform those who adopt these methods, both in speed (Hermelin
and O'Connor 1990) and complexity (Sacks 1985; Waterhouse 1988).
For example, in a pioneering empirical study, a mathematics graduate
trained in the appropriate algorithms took 11.46 seconds to generate
all the primes between integers 301 and 393 whereas a non-verbal
autistic young man who had not previously confronted such a task
took only 1.16 seconds (Hermelin and O'Connor 1990). Not only
was the savant ten times faster, but he also made far fewer errors.
Importantly, no practically realizable algorithm has yet been
invented for rapidly identifying primes in excess of 8 figures
as apparently performed by the autistic savant twins (Sacks 1985).
It would be interesting to compare the active (functional) brain
images of autistic savant calculators with those of individuals
who calculate via learned algorithms. We might anticipate significant
differences between the two, possibly analogous to those between
native and second language performance as recently observed by
Hirsch's group (Kim, Relkin, Lee and Hirsch 1997). Our native
language is acquired unconsciously, whereas second language acquisition
is hard work. Accordingly, the arithmetic ability of autistic
savants could be functionally like that of a native language whereas
it is expected to be more like a second language in most of us.
5. What is required for arithmetic calculations?
Apart from learning the nomenclature or the symbolic representation
of numbers, integer arithmetic is simply the ability to separate
groups into an equal number of elements - that is to equipartition.
For example, 12 elements can be represented as two equal groups
of 6 elements or 4 equal groups of 3 elements. Equipartitioning
may also be pertinent to another common skill of autistic savants
- calendar calculating - where the day of the week is given upon
being presented with any date, say 1000 years in the past or future
(Sacks 1985; Hermelin and O'Connor 1986; Treffert 1989; Young,
R.L. and Nettlebeck, T. 1994). We surmise from this that equipartitioning
is fundamental to some yet unknown aspect of mental processing. It is intriguing to contemplate which aspect, analytical or perceptual.
The actual method of calculation, while intriguing, is not central
to our thesis. Rather, our hypothesis rests on the very existence
of an ability to do lightning calculations without training. Perhaps
mathematical savants tap a mental process which spatially represents
groups and patterns (Welling 1994) and equipartitions them analogous
to the mathematical procedure of factorizing. This could also
explain why primes to mathematical savants are the odd man out
in groups of numbers and are reacted to as if they are very peculiar
indeed (Hermelin and O'Connor 1990). Others have suggested the
possibility of savants using modular arithmetic (Sacks 1985; Steward
1975).
Fig 3 Generating primes: to determine whether it is has
any factors other than 1 or itself. We need only consider prime
factors less than the square root of the number. There are well
known tricks for rapidly determining whether a number is divisible
by 2, 3, 5 or 11, but in general it is necessary to divide by
each prime. However if we are testing a sequence of consecutive
numbers it is not necessary to test every number separately by
dividing by each prime. Once you have determined that a number
is divisible by 7 then you know every 7th number thereafter is
divisible by 7. This gives us an alternative way to find primes,
one known in antiquity, that does not explicitly involve division.
Above we have adapted this ancient method to find the primes in
the range 101-120 (row 1). First we eliminate the even numbers
and numbers ending in 5 (row 2). Dividing 101 by 3 leaves a remainder
of 2, revealing 102 as the first multiple of 3. We then eliminate
every third number starting with 102 (row 3). Finally , dividing
101 by 7 leaves a remainder of 3, revealing 105 as the first multiple
of 7. We then eliminate every seventh number starting with 105
(row 4). Since we need only consider prime factors less than the
square root of 121 equals 11, the remaining numbers are all prime.
Memory and algorithms (learned or induced) are known to play
a crucial role in the techniques employed by lightning calculators
from the normal population (Smith 1983; Dehaene 1997). But the
beautiful work of Anderson, O'Connor and Hermelin (in press) has
ruled out the role of memory for savant calculators. They also
found that the performance profile of the savant calculator closely
matched that of the control who was using the Eratosthenes algorithm
for identifying primes as suggested earlier by Hermelin and O'Connor
(1990). But it remains possible that other strategies for finding
primes (see Fig. 3), not all of which need be arithmetic, could
also have similar performance profiles. Whatever the case, this
suggests that savant calculators have privileged access to some
form of algorithmic mental processing.
6. Music and number
Some autistic savants have the ability to keep time for extended
periods with accuracy to the second (Treffert 1989). Apparently,
our internal clocks are more precise than might have been imagined.
For example, when one autistic child was awakened he said, "It's
2.14 AM", then he went back to sleep (Rimland and Fein 1988,
p 485). This ability to equipartition time could also contribute
to the impressive musical skills of many autistic savants (Treffert
1989, Hill 1978; Miller 1989) and, when coupled with equipartitioning
of space, could suggest a mechanism which interrelates music and
mathematics.
There no doubt are other surprises that can be revealed by those
"abnormal" minds which are somehow aware of interim
mental processes and information not normally available through
introspection. For example, is it possible that the senses of
normal individuals are mixed and accessible for comparison in
a way that individuals with synaesthesia (Luria 1987; Cytovic
and Wood 1982) might suggest?
7. Discussion
As Howe (1989, p. 83) so aptly puts it, 'We experience only "the
whole": it takes the evidence provided by unusual people
in whom mental integration is incomplete owing to retardation,
brain damage, or some other kind of mental "disturbance"
to make us appreciate how smooth functioning of a person's total
mental system depends on the parts, or sub-systems, that underlie
it.' This philosophy underpins our present investigation. In particular,
we believe that savants offer a window into "lower"
level information used to construct our percepts and our judgements.
From this we have argued that some mental processing exists in
us all, for purposes yet unknown, which is recruited by autistic
lightning calculators to perform precise integer arithmetic calculations,
such as multiplication, division, factoring and identifying primes.
This highly quantitative numerical ability of autistic lightning
calculators is in sharp contrast with the well known qualitative
sense of numerosity displayed by human babies and even animals
(Gallistel and Gelman 1992; Gallistel 1990; Wynn 1992; Wynn 1995;
Dehaene 1997). For example, babies and animals can estimate the
number of objects in a collection with an error that is proportional
to the number itself. This turns out to be accurate for perceiving
and estimating 1, 2 or 3 objects but is grossly inaccurate for
judging large numbers. As Dehaene 1997 (p 119) concludes in his
authoritative overview, "An innate sense of approximate numerical
quantities may well be imbedded in our genes; but when faced with
exact symbolic calculation we lack the resources".
Our paper is concerned with mathematical savants. Now, savants
are far more prevalent in the autistic population than in any
other group and also it is among this group where multiple savant
skills most frequently occur (Rimland and Fein 1988; Triffert
1989). But, only a small fraction of the total autistic population
are savants. And, this fraction tends to be predominately composed
of those with early infantile autism (Treffert 1989), a condition
first described by Kanner (1943). Our theoretical perspective
is derived from savants in this category, although it could apply
to savants in general. We believe that savants with early infantile
autism have privileged access to lower levels of information and
that they are impoverished in concept formation, compared with
the general autistic population (Charman and Baron-Cohen 1993).
With maturity certain concepts can be acquired, but often at the
loss or reduction of their savant skills (Selfe 1977; Smith 1983).
It is worth mentioning, as have mathematicians of acclaim (Hadamard
1949), that savant lightning calculations are idiosyncratic and
not representative of what would normally be considered a mathematical
talent. Mathematicians are primarily concerned with the conceptual,
whereas autistic savants have extreme difficulty with learning
even the simplest mathematical concepts. In this regard it is
interesting that Baron-Cohen and his colleagues (1997) have recently
found that fathers and grandfathers of children with autism were
more than twice as often in the field of engineering than were
fathers and grandfathers of normal children. Similarly, they found
that autism occurred significantly more often in families of students
in the field of physics, engineering and mathematics (Baron-Cohen
et al. 1998). Although these studies address the general autistic
population and are not restricted to savants, they are nonetheless
fascinating and deserve further investigation.
Prevalent explanations for savant skills: Mathematical
savants have fascinated their investigators through the centuries
(Smith 1985, Treffert 1989) so it should be of no surprise that
various theories have been advanced to explain the phenomenon.
These are discussed in depth elsewhere (Treffert 1989; Nettelbeck
and Young in press). We critique the conceptual thrust of the
most prominent views so that they can be contrasted with the perspective
leading to our claim that integer arithmetic is fundamental to
mental processing. Essentially there are two popular explanations
for savant skills: one holds that obsessive focussed learning
promotes savant skills just as it does for any expertise: the
other postulates that genius and savants alike have highly developed
domain specific neural structures (innate talent). In contrast,
our view is that the mechanism for savant skills resides equally
in us all but that (without some abnormality like autism) it can
not normally be accessed for the skill in question.
Mathematical savants arise from obsessive learning: Many
authors believe that the extraordinary feats of lightning calculators
are a consequence of their passion and preoccupation for learning
mathematics in much the same way it is for truly innovative mathematicians,
see for example, e.g. Smith (1983) and Rimland and Fein (1988).
Dehaene (1997, p. 164) especially presents a compelling discussion,
concluding that, "A talent for calculation thus seems to
arise more from precocious training, often accompanied by an exceptional
or even pathological capacity to concentrate on the narrow domain
of numbers, than from an innate gift". Howe (1989 p. 150)
agrees, "The circumstances that give rise to a retarded savant's
achievements are not entirely different from those in which a
person of normal or above average intelligence chooses to specialize
in a particular area of interest".
Savants have better brains for arithmetic: Another view
holds that both genius and savant alike are endowed with exceptional
domain specific neural structures (innate talent) which promote
their specialized skills. This view also has a number of distinguished
advocates. For example, O'Connor (1989, p. 19) says "But
just as there are specialized centres mediating speech, so there
may be centres for calculation, graphic skills or music. One can
suffer deficits in these abilities so why not also have specific
gifts!" Ramachandran (1998, p. 197) enriches the story further,
speculating about savants, "that some specialized brain regions
may have become enlarged at the expense of others", e.g.
the angular gyrus for mathematical talent. Howe (1989, p. 153),
also suggests that the savant artist Nadia and the man with the
seemingly perfect memory, Shereskeskii, might fall into this category.
While the obsessive learning and the better brains theory for
savant arithmetic may not be mutually exclusive (Hermelin and
O'Connor 1990) and while they have their compelling aspects, we
find them improbable for the following reasons: Those who have
protracted experience with savants frequently report that the
core ability behind the skill emerges "spontaneously" and does not improve qualitatively with time even though it might
become better articulated (O'Connor 1989; Selfe 1977; Treffert
1989). This argues against obsessive learning. Furthermore, from
the perspective of either theory, it would appear highly coincidental
that such a peculiar subset of mathematics should be so compelling
to a significant fraction of autistic savants across all cultures,
and also that many of these same savants simultaneously have several
savant skills (Rimland and Fein 1988) each of which are similarly
peculiar and restricted. And why is there little or no invention
or creative component in the skill? All of this mitigates against
either obsessive learning or better brains being a plausible explanation
for mathematical savants, as does the fact that savant skills
can even arise after an accident or illness in otherwise normal
individuals (Treffert 1989).
The mechanisms for savant mathematics reside equally in us
all but can not normally be accessed: Now in contrast to the
popular views discussed above, the unique aspect of our perspective
is that the mechanism and information drawn on for savant mathematics resides equally in us all but it can not be recruited by
us for mathematics. In other words, we believe that mathematical
savants, like all autistic savants, arise from their privileged
access to lower levels of raw information. Their skills
are essentially a form of mimicry, and thus naturally lead to
drawing, perfect pitch, time telling, astonishing recall, hyperlexia,
echolalia, etc. Hence, the very same peculiar savant skills appear
across different cultures. However, unlike drawing and perfect
pitch, we do not know what lower levels are recruited for savant
mathematical skills. But, we hypothesize that savant mathematics
is propelled by some fundamental mechanism which equipartitions
- possibly in both space and time. Why is it that savants have
privileged access to lower levels of information? Perhaps it is
promoted by a loss of those centres that control executive or
integrative mechanisms as elaborated on by Treffert (1989) and
also by Baron-Cohen (1995) in relation to the fact that individuals
with autism lack a theory of mind. This in turn could leave savants
less concept driven (Snyder and Thomas, 1997; Snyder 1998), or
as Frith (1989) argues, lacking central coherence (Pring, Hermelin
and Heavey 1995; Heaton, Hermelin and Pring 1998).
An intriguing question remains. Although we do not normally have
access to the lower levels of information as do savants, is there
nonetheless some artificial means to promote this access, say
via induced altered states of consciousness? Possibly pertinent
to this suggestion is the fact mentioned above that savant skills
have been known to follow a severe physical illness, an operation,
or a near drowning (Treffert 1989). This reinforces our belief
that savant skills are innate in us all but are normally suppressed.
So in conclusion, we believe that the mental apparatus to perform
"lightning fast" integer arithmetic calculations such
as multiplication and division resides in us all, even though
it is not normally accessible. The brain appears to perform something
tantamount to arithmetic calculations (or analogously equipartitioning)
for some unknown aspect of mental processing. The challenge now
is to unravel which aspect.
We appreciate the critical insights of Mike Anderson, Kirsty
Galloway McLean, Ted Nettlebeck, Mandy Thomas and Robyn Young.
References
Anderson, M., O'Connor, N. and Hermelin, B. (in press) A specific
calculating ability. Intelligence.
Asperger, H. 1944 Die "Autistischen Psychopathen"
in Kindersalter. Archiv für Psychiatrie and Nervenkrankherten 117, 76-136. English translation in: V. Frith, 1991 Autism
and Asperger Syndrome. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Baron-Cohen, S. 1995 Mindblindness. Cambridge, M.A.: MIT
Press.
Baron-Cohen, S., Bolton, P., Wheelwright, S., Short, L., Mead.
G., Smith, A. and Scahill, V. 1998 Autism occurs more often in
families of physicists, engineers and mathematicians. Autism, 2, 296-301.
Baron-Cohen, S. Wheelwright, S., Stott, C., Bolton, P. and Goodyer,
I. 1997 Is there a link between engineering and autism? Autism, 1, 101-109.
Barnes, R.C. and Earnshaw, S.M. 1995 Problems with the savant
syndrome: A brief case study. British Journal of Learning Disabilities, 23, 124-126.
Bartlett, F.C. 1932 Remembering: A study in experimental and
social psychology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Charman, T. and Baron-Cohen, S. 1993 Drawing development in autism:
The intellectual to visual realism shift. British Journal of
Developmental Psychology, 11, 171-185.
Cytovic, R.E. and Wood, F.B. 1982 Synaesthesia II: Psychophysical
relationships in the synaesthesia of geometrically shaped taste
and colored hearing. Brain and Cognition, 1, 23-35.
Dehaene, S. 1997 The Number Sense. New York: Oxford.
Frith, V. 1989 Autism: Explaining the enigma. Oxford:
Blackwell.
Gallistel, C.R. 1990 The Organizaiton of Learning, Chapter
10. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Gallistel, C.R. and Gelman, R. 1992 Preverbal and verbal counting
and computation. Cognition, 44,43-74.
Gombrich, E.H. 1960 Art and Illusion. Oxford: Phaidon
Press.
Hadamard, J. 1949 The Psychology of Invention in the Mathematical
Field, p 58. New York: Dover.
Heaton, P., Hermelin, B. and Pring, L. 1998 Autism and pitch
processing: a precursor for savant musical ability? Music Perception, Spring 15 (2) 291-305.
Hemholtz, H. 1910 Handbuck der physiologischen optic, vol 111 Hamburg: Leopald Voss.
Hermelin, B. and O'Connor, N. 1990 Factors and primes: a specific
numerical ability. Psychological Medicine 20, 163-189.
Hermelin, B. and O'Connor, N. 1986 Idiot savants calendrical
calculators: Rules and regularities. Psychological Medicine 16, 885-893.
Hill, A.L. 1978 Mentally retarded individuals with special
skills. In International Review of Research in Mental Retardation,
277- 298 vol. 9. (ed. N.R. Eller). New York: Academic Pan.
Howe, M.J.A. 1989 Fragments of Genius. London and New
York: Routledge.
Kanner, L. 1943 Autistic disturbances of affective contact.
Nervous child 2, 217-250.
Kim, K.H.S., Relkin, N.R., Lee, Kyoung-Min and Hirsch, J. 1997
Distinct cortical areas associated with native and second languages. Nature, 388, 171-174.
Luria, A.R. 1987 The mind of a mnemonist. Cambridge, Mass:
Harvard University Press.
Miller, L. 1989 Musical savants: Exceptional skills in the
mentally retarded. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Myers, F.W.H. 1903 Human Personality and its Survival of Bodily
Death. New York: Longmans and Green.
Nettlebeck, T. and Young, R. In press Savant syndrome. In
International Review of Research in Mental Retardation, vol. 22.
(ed. Larraine Glidden). New York: Academic Press. (In press).
O'Connor, N.O. 1989 The performance of the "idiot-savant":
implicit and explicit. British Journal of Disorders of communication 24, 1-20.
Penfield W. and Roberts, L. 1966 Speech and Brain Mechanisms. New York: Atheneum.
Pring, L., Hermelin, B. and Heavey, L. 1995 Savants, segments,
art and autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 36, 1065-1076.
Profita, J. and Biddes, I.G. 1988 Perfect Pitch. American
Journal of Medical Genetics 29, 763-771.
Ramachandran, V.S. 1998 Phantoms in the brain. New York:
William Morrow.
Rimland, B. and Fein, D. 1988 Special Talents of Autistic
Savants in the Exceptional Brain 472-492 (ed. Obler, L.K.
and Fein, D.) New York: The Guilford Press.
Sacks, O. 1985 The Man Who Mistook His Wife for a Hat.
London:Duckworth.
Selfe, L. 1977 Nadia: A Case of Extraordinary Drawing Ability
in Children. London: Academic Press.
Smith, S.B. 1983 The Great Mental Calculators, the Psychology,
Methods and Lines of Calculating Prodigies, Past and Present. New York: Columbia University Press.
Snyder, A.W. and Barlow, H.B. 1988 Human vision: Revealing the
artist's touch. Nature 331, 117-118 .
Snyder, A.W. and Thomas, M. 1997 Autistic artists give clues
to cognition. Perception 26, 93-96.
Snyder, A.W. 1998 Breaking Mindsets. Mind and Language 13,1-10.
Stewart, I. 1975 Concepts of Modern Mathematics. New York:
Harmondsworth.
Sullivan, R.C. 1992 Rain Man and Joseph. In High-functioning
Individuals with Autism. 243-251 (ed. E. Schopler
and G.B. Mesibov). New York: Plenum Press.
Takeuchi, A.H. and Hulse, S.H. 1992 Absolute pitch. Psychological
Bulletin 113(2), 345-361.
Treffert, D.A. 1989 Extraordinary People. New York: Harper
and Row.
Waterhouse, L. 1988 Extraordinary visual memory and pattern
perception in an autistic boy. In The Exceptional Brain, Neuropsychology
of Talent and Special Abilities 348-356(ed. L.K. Obler and D.
Fein ). New York: The Guilford Press.
Welling, H. 1994 Prime number identificators in idiot savants:
can they calculate them? Journal of Autism and Developmental
Disorder 24, 199-207.
Wiltshire, S. 1987 Drawings. London: Dent and Sons.
Wynn, K. 1992 Addition and subtraction by human infants. Nature, 358, 749-750.
Wynn, K. 1995 Origins of numerical knowledge. Mathematical
Cognition, 1, 35-60.
Young, R.L. and Nettlebeck, T. 1994 The "intelligence"
of calendrical calculators. American J |